This week concludes our debate on exit polls in
Election 2004. Tune in next week, when letters from our
readers re-emerge.
Oh, dear. Rebutting a rebuttal . Seldom a
constructive use of time. But then, to
ignore Steve Freeman’s published comments about my
TomPaine.com Ohio election research would be to acquiesce in
the ongoing degradation of serious debate over real problems
that bedevil the nation’s elections apparatus—and to endorse
this distraction from fashioning viable systemic
solutions.
Freeman is a leading proponent of the theory that the
election in Ohio was deliberately stolen, and that proof of
this lies both in a gap between exit poll results and final
tallies, and in anecdotes of election day irregularities. In
my article, I expressed doubts about Freeman’s argument, and
in his rebuttal, he dismissed what I had to say.
I won’t go into everything Freeman says, although
everything he says is either partially or completely wrong.
But I will focus on a few key areas. (If you didn’t see his
piece, you can find it here. You can also find my original
piece here or near the top of my own
website, http://www.russbaker.com/.)
First of all: credibility. Freeman, a university professor,
makes much of his academic credentials. Freeman writes: “A
study of election integrity…requires an understanding of
election practices and voting systems, and, most importantly,
an ability and willingness to investigate a complex subject in
which the data and the accompanying official pronouncements
are themselves suspect. I hold degrees in both political
science and systems science, and have received four national
awards for best research paper of the year—on four different
topics in three different fields.”
I won’t explore here the limitations of some of my own
cocksure college professors, other than to say that academia
has as many bad eggs as good, and that mere titles, positions
and accolades don’t necessarily qualify someone to declare an
election stolen.
Freeman, who selectively cites favorable journalists as
credible authorities, seems to summarily ignore my own long,
unsullied record of journalistic accuracy. The fact is, absent
a Ph.D., one can do pretty well in getting to the bottom of
things with any or all of the following: a keen mind, a good
nose, a sense of fairness and healthy skepticism, and the
ability to apply logic and ask good questions.
Probably the most disturbing part of Freeman’s theory is
his myopic understanding of the ways in which presidential
exit polls function. He writes: “Baker dismisses the validity
of exit polls, but prominent survey researchers…political
scientists…and journalists concur that they are highly
reliable. As far back as 1987, political columnist David
Broder wrote that exit polls "are the most useful analytic
tool developed in my working life."
Actually, I never said that exit polls weren’t valid. I
said that they are imprecise. And I said that, in exploring
the particulars of what happened on November 2 with the people
who did the exit poll, I learned about all manner of technical
complication that could have affected the numbers and the
perceptions—a source of potential problems that Freeman fails
to deal with.
The outfit that did the exit polling faced myriad
headaches—including apparent quality-control issues with some
of their canvassers whose job was to convince voters to
voluntarily complete surveys outside polling places. Many of
them are hired through subcontractors; some are more
scrupulous in following rules than others; some relate to
voters better than others and therefore perhaps get more
accurate information. Probably the key factor was the
election-day environment in which those people operated—almost
circuslike, often combative, with teams of election watchers
camped outside polling places wearing NAACP Election
Protection T-shirts. It’s foolish or disingenuous to assert
the accuracy of exit polls while denying the probability that
some Republicans may have felt disinclined to say that they
voted for Bush.
The pollsters intend to do what they can to rectify these
matters in the future, but they underline a fundamental
reality about exit polls—they are surveys. They are not exact
replications of actual voting.
Suffice to say that if the author of some study concedes
that it was flawed—and if he is considered generally expert
and credible—as is true of exit pollster pere Warren
Mitofsky—then there is no reason to insist that he is covering
up some hideous plot. As it happens, besides being universally
trusted and respected, the pollster is personally a lifelong
liberal who had no use for Bush.
Freeman et al miss another key reality about exit
polls. You can’t say that the exit poll results were right, in
that there are no actual “results.” The canvassers working for
the exit pollsters go to selected precincts, not to all
precincts, and so whatever they report must then be massaged
through a variety of processes designed to correctly adjust
and extrapolate in order to ensure that reported totals
accurately reflect the will of the voters. Therefore, what the
Mitofsky people were doing was sending across, privately, to
their media clients, a flow of numbers reflecting different
calculations and variables. The numbers that Freeman and his
acolytes have spread, virus-like, through the Internet are but
a small drink from a steady, evolving stream from
Mitofsky.
Most importantly, exit polls aren’t intended to be a check
against election fraud. They are attempts to predict the final
outcome of the voting. In order to improve the accuracy of
their predictions, the pollsters constantly revise them until
they are in conformity with an emerging actual tally of the
real vote.
Instead of embracing this essential truth about how exit
polls work (or don’t work), Freeman cites other people who
believe them to be largely accurate. And indeed they are.
Throughout the day, the Ohio numbers being produced by
Mitofsky were only off by a few points, which made a big
difference only because of the closeness of the Bush-Kerry
race in that state.
Freeman moves on to cite evidence that the election was
stolen. Principally, he credits a “commission” headed
by Rep. John Conyers. But that “commission” didn’t
extensively investigate first hand. Mostly it just took
statements from voters and activists, statements that were not
fully vetted for their accuracy or for the likelihood that
specific cases could be broadly extrapolated. Clearly, there
were many odd and troubling incidents, but the reality is that
when you carefully tally up all of the best-documented
irregularities, it is still not enough to reverse Bush’s Ohio
victory. Even some leading figures in contesting the
Ohio results concede that point.
Certainly, as I said in my original TomPaine piece, some
Republican officials, including Secretary of State J. Kenneth
Blackwell, behaved in an overtly partisan and inappropriate
manner. An investigation of his conduct is warranted. But
there’s no evidence that Blackwell’s rulings actually resulted
in a reversal of the election.
Critics of the electoral mechanism (and I am certainly one
of them) need to be very careful not to overshoot. It doesn’t
do anyone any good to throw bad arguments at a good cause. Or
to dispirit and needlessly anger people by persuading them
that their vote was rendered meaningless by broad-based fraud,
when you really don’t know that to be the case. The full truth
is not yet clear, but promoting sensational scenarios does
little to serve the public interest.
As Norman Mailer put it so well in a recent essay for the
London Sunday Times , “Good hypotheses depend on real
questions, which is to say questions that do not always
generate happy answers.”